April 15, 2012, was the centennial of the sinking of the Titanic. Coincidentally, five days before that centennial, two authors posted online a working paper, “Every man for himself: Gender, Norms and Survival in Maritime Disasters.” The next day their employer described that working paper in a press release entitled “Titanic is an Exception among Disasters at Sea.” By the next day, the Associated Press (AP) was distributing this news under the title “Researchers: Titanic was an exception, male {sic} chivalry on sinking ships is ‘a myth’.” The Washington Post, the NY Daily News, the Globe and Mail (Toronto, Canada), and other leading news sources ran the AP article on that same day. By the centennial of the Titanic’s sinking, about 300 online news sources from Malta to India to Australia were running this news.
In reporting this news, major news organizations mislead their readers, presented absurd statements, and furthered grotesque sexism. The news stories reported that “two Swedish researchers … economists Mikael Elinder and Oscar Erixon of Uppsala University” did an “82-page study.” That “study” was the working paper posted online five days before the Titanic’s centennial. The main text of the working paper spans six pages. The news reports described the working paper as if it were objective, authoritative scholarly research. To encourage readers to remain ignorant of its credibility, none of the news stories linked to the working paper. News reports thus presented as scholarly research what would be better understood as a tendentious media gambit.
Recognizing grave weaknesses in the story of the working paper / press release should not have been beyond the capabilities of a professional journalist. For example, the press release states:
It is expected that the crew should rescue passengers, but our results show that captains and crew are more likely to survive than passengers.
That’s a non sequitur. Consider the relationship between the two independent clauses. Is it necessary for the captain and crew to be more likely to die in order for them to work to rescue passengers? Try to think of any other factors besides helping passengers that would be relevant to the differential survival of the captain and crew (hint: experience at sea and swimming ability). The bottom of the news report includes a quote from a ship captain making this obvious point. The news story presents the captain’s statement merely as another viewpoint, rather than as reasoning that seriously undermines the value of paying any attention to the working-paper’s “findings.”
The news article presented statistics to impress and factually dominate the reader, but not statistics that usefully and accurately inform. See if you notice a problem with these two statistical statements from the news article:
- Out of the 15,000 people who died in the 18 accidents, only 17.8 percent of the women survived compared with 34.5 percent of the men.
- Of the 1,496 people that perished with the Titanic, 73.3 percent of the women and 50.4 percent of the children survived compared to only 20.7 percent of the men.
These statistics make no sense. How could a share of the persons who died have survived? The above first sentence of statistical gibberish apparently is based on these sentences in the working paper:
Table 2 reports tests of each of the 6 hypotheses conducted in separate regressions, as well as together in one regression. We find that the survival rate of women is 16.7 percentage points lower than, or about half of (17.8% vs. 34.5%), that of men.[1]
Here a survival rate is described as a percentage apparently related to hypotheses tested in regressions. The meaningfulness of those numbers depend on a vast array of other assumptions and specifications in the working paper’s analysis. Most of those assumptions were not statistically supported. In short, the working paper is also filled with statistical gibberish.[2]
full set of sinking ships
|
sinking ships authoritatively discriminating against men
|
|||||
persons on ships
|
persons perished
|
% perished
|
persons on ships
|
persons perished
|
% perished
|
|
males
|
10,350
|
6,240
|
60%
|
4,207
|
2,973
|
71%
|
females |
4,794
|
3,237
|
68%
|
1,150
|
592
|
51%
|
Those interested in the truth start with understanding simple facts. While the working paper failed to provide a summary table of deaths by sex, such a table can be constructed from the working paper with some additional work. In the working paper’s set of sinking ships, 60% of the males and 68% of the females on the sinking ships died. To what extent does that death-share difference reflect differences in seafaring experience and relevant physical skills? The working paper didn’t evaluate that issue. Even if men did privilege women on sinking ships, a higher share of men could still survive if they had more sea experience and better swimming ability.
Nearly twice as many males died on the sinking ships as did females. That fact points to sex-differentiated exposure to risk. Women have been shielded from significant risks to which men are subject. For example, in contrast to wide-ranging efforts to promote gender equity in the workplace, men still predominate in the most dangerous jobs. In the U.S. in 2006, the dead person in 92% of workplace fatalities was a man. Among U.S. soldiers on active duty in 2008, men outnumbered women by about six to one. Among U.S. soldiers killed in Iraq, men outnumbered women about forty-two to one.
Authoritative discrimination against men on sinking ships is associated with a higher share of men perishing and a lower share of women perishing. For those sinking ships in which the captain ordered that women and children go first into the lifeboats, 71% of the males perished, while 51% of the females perished. On sinking ships in which the captain is known not to have ordered sex discrimination for the evacuation, the shares perishing were nearly the reverse: 50% of males and 70% of females. Thus an accurate and meaningful finding can be uncovered: on sinking ships, authoritative sex discrimination against men has deadly effects on men.[3]
The working paper and the press release presented these facts much differently. Following a similar statement in the working paper, the press release declared:
On the ships where the captain gave the order ‘women and children first’, the difference in survival rates between men and women is lower. But women survived to a higher extent than men only when this order was enforced by the threat of violence.
This indicates an important role of leaders in the face of disasters. It is, however, unusual for captains to give such an order. [4]
The European Union was explicitly founded as a society in which “non-discrimination … and equality between women and men prevail.”[5] Uppsala University in Sweden seems to support leaders threatening violence against men to uphold sex discrimination against men on sinking ships. Underscoring its commitment to sexism and anti-male bigotry, the working paper’s title describes not privileging women and children as “every man for himself.”
In a disaster, many ordinary men may have a gut sense of their equal worth as human beings. They may act accordingly. That doesn’t mean that men would not or do not help some others deserving of help. But, in extraordinary circumstances, many ordinary men may reject their categorical disposability.
In ordinary circumstances, men tend to accept their disposibility without protest. That’s modern chivalry. The communicative success of the Uppsala University working paper testifies to the enduring significance of such chivalry.
Universities and major media can help to guide societies to the secure shores of justice and truth. Many today, especially in Europe, are now far out at sea. Don’t follow an inhumane and untruthful order into the blue void. Seek your own saving way!
Data: set of sinking ships, with characteristics and sex-differentiated death totals (Excel version)
Read more:
- sexism and chivalry in the Costa Concordia sinking
- recognizing sexist values around the world
- sex differences in social communication
Notes:
[1] Elinder & Erixson (2012) p. 6.
[2] The quality of the statistical work in Elinder & Erixson (2012) appears to me to be so low as to not merit attention. Its hypothesis are laughably ill-formed, its framework of hypothesis testing is not credible (do you really believe that those hypotheses were formulated independent of the data? What does that imply for the interpretation of statistical tests?), the results are incompletely reported, the data have not been posted, the model forms chosen are not well justified and tested, etc. The main value of Elinder & Erixson (2012) is to intimidate untrained readers and obscure simple facts. While Elinder & Erixson (2012) has not undergone peer review, many peer-review publications present false results. The institutional problems that Begley & Ellis (2012) identify in preclinical cancer research are even worse in economic research.
[3] The data in Elinder & Erixson (2012) do not consistently distinguish adults and children. Because the number of young children is probably relatively small, males and females are reasonable proxies for men and women.
[4] In their concluding main section entitled “Discussion,” Elinder & Erixson (2012), p. 8, states:
Most notably, we find that it seems as if {sic} it is the policy of the captain, rather than the moral sentiments of men, that determines if women are given preferential treatment in shipwrecks. This suggests an important role for leaders in disasters.
[5] See Treaty of the European Union, Article 2.
References:
Begley, C. Glenn, and Lee M. Ellis. 2012. “Drug development: Raise standards for preclinical cancer research.” Nature. 483 (7391): 531-533.
Elinder, Mikael and Oscar Erixson. 2012. “Every man for himself: Gender, Norms and Survival in Maritime Disasters.” Uppsala Universitet, Department of Economics, Working Paper 2012:8.
Update: The Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America (PNAS) received the paper for review on May 2, 2012, approved it on June 29, 2012, and published it on July 30, 2012. It was published as Elinder, Mikael and Oscar Erixson. 2012. “Gender, social norms, and survival in maritime disasters.” doi:10.1073/pnas.1207156109 The publication in PNAS generated another round of coverage in major news sources. While the second round of press reporting seems to have avoided obvious statistical blunders, the reporting provided no critical perspective on the article’s claims.
The peer review of the paper did little to improve it. The criticisms above still apply. PNAS published online a supporting dataset with the article. The organization of the dataset, which shows no regard for informative data structure, is further reason to doubt the article’s tendentious statistical analysis.
Here’s an example of the article’s scientific veneer:
A small survival disadvantage for women is difficult to interpret, as it can either indicate that the WCF norm has helped women from a potentially larger disadvantage or that the norm has not been upheld. However, if we observe a substantial survival disadvantage of women we regard it as evidence that compliance with the WCF norm is exceptional in maritime disasters. (p. 1)
Note the technical-normative babble “compliance with the WCF norm” and “the norm has not been upheld.” The actual issue is whether passengers and crew valued women’s lives and children’s lives above men’s lives. This paper strives to make that gender inequality into a norm for which society demands compliance. In the historical shipwreck sample, men are likely to differ significantly from women in sea-survival skills and physical capabilities. With regard to isolating the quantitative extent to which men helping women affects men’s and women’s differential survival, the article offers little actual quantitative evidence. Instead, the article states: “if we observe a substantial survival disadvantage of women we regard it as evidence…” The observed facts aren’t actually evidence of the authors’ claim; the authors merely “regard it as evidence.” I regard this article as evidence that junk statistical work, if appropriately tendentious, gets published quickly in PNAS.
The European Union is currently teetering on the edge of an economic disaster that could bring down the entire world economy. Why are European economists Mikael Elinder and Oscar Erixon publishing about gender survival rates in maritime disasters? The economies of Southern Europe are often compared to sinking ships but that can’t be the explanation. The only explanation I can imagine is they’re hoping to transfer into the Women’s Studies department at Uppsala University. Why would they want to do that? Perhaps they think Women’s Studies offers better job security in an economic collapse.
As i understand, the point of this paper was to convey that whether WCF was as prevalent as we percieve it to be, not whether women or men have better surviving skill. In that regard, the papers proved what the abstract and newspapers say.
There are papers that have examined how widespread certain social mores actually were, these are valid topics.
“the point of this paper was to convey that whether WCF was as prevalent as we percieve it to be”: that’s a strawperson. The article provides no objective measure of “how prevalent we perceive it to be.” Getting such an objective measure would require a representative survey.
By the authors’ own data, about a third of men on sinking ships were ordered to privilege women and children (WCF). Even without an authoritative order, WCF probably operated in practice in a significant share of additional cases. The absurd press coverage and the rhetoric of the “scholarly” paper itself underscores social support for men gender-privileging women on sinking ships.
WCF, like “It’s unmanly to hit women” in case of violence, is quite a popular phrase. I don’t see how a lack of survey calls for the whole point to be regarded as strawman given how popular it is.
The authors showed a survival advantage and disadvantage of women when WCF order was given and wasn’t given. The gap is significant. Again, since this was about probing how true a popular perception is, that WCF orders were given in one-third of cases wouldn’t disqualify the conclusion that maritime disasters were more characterized by people vying to save themselves than saving the women and children or the passengers, even in case of administrative orders since one-third is significantly less than two-thirds.
The author’s conclusion and the most visible press coverage was about what the more proper characterization of maritime disaster is once you analyze them. To me, that seems to be neutral on what should have happened.
You’re missing how the article is manipulating readers. It’s not “neutral on what should have happened.” The orientation of the article is to minimize the significance of life-denying discrimination against men. It could say that authoritative orders discriminating against men on sinking ships are less common than popularly thought. Or it could say that authoritative orders discriminating against men occurred on fully one-third of sinking ships. The second statement is more factually precise and more morally significant.
“The authors showed a survival advantage and disadvantage of women when WCF order was given and wasn’t given. The gap is significant. ” That’s not surprising. Put differently, authoritative discrimination against men increases men’s death rate. Don’t you see that it matters the way one phrases the same obvious effect?